Humans are able to see things that are both far and near, and can actually identify where those objects are in space (meaning, they can determine if those objects are close or far away). This sort of depth perception requires both of our eyes, which is referred to as binocular cues (de...
(1960) The perception of a depth interval with binocular disparity cues. J. Psychol. 50: pp. 257-269Gogel, W. C, 1960, "The perception of a depth interval with.binocular disparity cues", Journal of Psychology, 50, 257-269.Gogel W C, 1960 "The perception of depth interval with ...
Although it is well known that motion-in-depth can be detected using binocular cues, it is not known whether those cues can be used to judge the speed of an object moving in depth. There are at least t two possible binocular cues that could be used by the visual system to calculate th...
When we track an object moving in depth, our eyes rotate in opposite directions. This type of “disjunctive” eye movement is called horizontal vergence. The sensory control signals for vergence arise from multiple visual cues, two of which, changing bin
and combined depth cues (Ban et al., 2012). The lack of fMRI activation of V5/MT is puzzling when compared with the neurophysiological recordings. Cortical area V5/MT, mostly studied in relation to the processing of visual motion, has proved to have an important role in binocular stereoscopic...
Thus, it has been argued (e.g., Julesz, 1971) that binocular vision may have evolved in predator species to help them break the camouflage of their prey by making use of interocular depth cues. However, it is still far from clear how this feat is accomplished by the mammalian visual ...
In summary, non-stereoscopic cues encompass monocular cues and at least four potential binocular non-stereoscopic cues: binocular luster, diplopia/confusion, binocular rivalry, and rivaldepth (Table1). In theory, any or all of these cues could be used to identify shapes or items in depth in ra...
According to cue- conflict theory, the pairing between the two discrepant cues results in the recalibration of disparity by the KDE such that a unit amount of disparity gives rise to less ap- parent depth than was perceived prior to exposure to the telestereoscope.! Alternatively, it is ...
Perceiving in Depth; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2012; Volume 2. 42. Ono, H. On Wells' 1792 law of visual direction. Percept. Psychophys. 1981, 30, 403–406. [CrossRef] [PubMed] 43. Ono, H.; Mapp, A.P. A restatement of Wells-Hering's law of visual direction. ...
Thus, brain imaging supports utilization of both cues in motion in depth computations and indicates the existence of an area adjacent to MT+ specialized for motion in depth. In this regard, it is noteworthy that neurons in macaque MT register disparity information as well as motion information (...